ARTICLE INFO

Article Type

Original Research

Authors

Hadi   H. (*)
Mohammadyari   S. (1)






(*) Physical Education and Sport Science Department, Amin Police University, Tehran, Iran
(1) Physical Education and Sport Science Department, Imam Ali University, Tehran, Iran

Correspondence

Address: Amin Police University, Shahid Kharazi Highway, End of Shahid Hemto West Road ,Tehran, Iran
Phone: -
Fax: +98 (21) 48931376
amir.hadi1@gmail.com

Article History

Received:  November  21, 2018
Accepted:  January 20, 2019
ePublished:  March 19, 2019

BRIEF TEXT


Stature control is an integral part of most daily activities and sports movements and is a decisive indicator in assessing the performance of athletes [1, 2].

... [3-9]. One of the ways to increase performance is to use mental exercises that are used as a complement to physical exercises in order to achieve this principle. Since mental exercises do not require any special equipment, and the individual is able to repeat these exercises alone after training alone, performing mental exercises is the best way to reduce the cost of rehabilitation, rehabilitation and readiness of athletes [10]. On the one hand, research by researchers suggests that mental training, similar to physical exercise, improves the motor skills of individuals [11,12], and on the other hand, it has been shown that the same nervous mechanisms that participate in learning with physical activity, are also active during mental training [13]. Many studies have been done on the effect of mental training on improving motor skills since the 1930, whose results are consistent, indicating the effects of mental training on performance improvement [12]. Research on Kayak, tennis and volleyball and strength athletes is one of the studies conducted in this field [14-18]. One of the researches in this field is the research of Fayradar and Saydai, entitled "Effects of mental training on reducing lumbar lordosis and low back pain". The results confirm the significant effects of performing a period of mental training on lumbar anomalous repair and lower back pain in subjects [19]. ... [20-23].

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of balanced mental training on the control of posture in the male studens of Amin Police University.

This research is experimental.

This research was conducted in Amin Police University in 2016 among non-athlete male students who had no symptoms of lower limb injury, atrial fibrillation and injuries that resulted in their inability to perform research tests.

From the research community, 30 students were randomly selected and divided into 2 groups of experimental and control each with 15 students.

To assess the dynamic control of the subjects' stature, after 5-10 minutes of stretching the lower and limbs and smooth running, the Star Excursion Balance Test was used in 8 directions (anterior, lateral anterior, internal anterior, lateral, internal, posterior, lateral posterior, and internal posterior) was used [24]. The validity and reliability of this test has been confirmed by Gary for the estimation of dynamic stature control [25]. In order to perform reaching actin, the subject's superior leg was placed at the center of the drawn star which had 8 directions at 45 degrees, and the subject performed the reaching action with non-superior leg (for determining the superior leg of the subjects, the falling reflex was used). Also, to reduce the learning effect, after describing how the test was conducted for the subjects, 6 reaching action was performed randomly by each subject; then each subject performed 3 attempts in each direction (the directions were randomly determined by the researcher), whose mean recorded as a record and was normalized baded on the length of their leg [24]. The dynamic control of the stature of subjects was evaluated before and after the training period. During the training period (10 days and each day one session), the subjects of the training group performed the balanced mental training [26], and the subjects of the control group continued their normal daily activities, according to the previous routine. The program of the mental training group was such that all subjects in the training group arrived at 8 to 12 in the morning for mental training in a quiet and appropriate room designed for this purpose. The subject laid on the comfortable bed and closed his eyes. The duration of the exercise in each session was 12 minutes. The first 5 minutes was allocated for relaxation exercises to enhance relaxation, comfort and concentration of the subject to perform the mental exercises. Then, by asking the individual about whether he was ready to begin training and concentration, mental training lasted in the same state and closed eyes in 2 3-minute periods with a minute rest. Balanced mental practice of the experimental group subjects was that the subject imagined a balance sheet without any mental background and then he embodied that he placed the non-superior leg on it and, after a little touching the plate with his leg, he had been on that with the closed eyes and fastened hands, trying to maintain control of the postures. At the end of the 3 minutes, the alarm timer sounded the person to the end of the exercise. Prior to the beginning of the training period, this practice was demonstrated by the examiner to all subjects in the experimental group. After completion of the training period, the SEBT posttest was performed in the same environment on the experimental and control groups. To determine the mean and standard deviation of age, height, and weight of subjects and their access interval in eight directions for SEBT, descriptive statistics were used, and independent t-test was used to compare age, height, weight between two groups. Covariance analysis was used to measure the distance of reaching points of subjects in the two groups after applying the exercise program.

The mean age, weight, and height of the subjects were 20.64±1.58 years, 68.28±2.74 kilograms, and 176.13±5.41 centimeter, respectively (Table 1).There was a significant difference in the distance of learners` reaching point between the two groups in the posterior directions (Sig. = 0.001, F2 = 252.352), lateral posterior (Sig. = 0.001; F=211.62), internal posterior (Sig=0.0001; F=195.52), Internal (Sig=0.0001; F=41.358) and internal anterior (Sig. =0.001; F=72.781). However, no significant difference was observed at the distance of reaching the subjects in the two groups in the anterior (Sig.=0.152; F=78.647), lateral anterior (Sig.=0.095; F=158.28) and lateral (Sig.=0.218; F=142.631) directions (Fig. 1).

The results showed that the balanced mental training significantly improved the distance of reaching point in 5 directions of the SEBT (internal anterior, internal posterior, internal, lateral posterior, and posterior), and thus significantly improved the dynamic control of posture in non-athlete male students that these results are in accordance with the findings of to the Firadar and Saydai [19], Caming and Hall [14], Harwood et al. [20] and Gilot et al. [21], who consistently reported the positive effects of mental training on increasing performance of athletes. ... [27-29]. Some scholars believe that mental training plays only in the planning and designing of motion and does not play a role in the performance phase and believe that during mental training, no activity takes place in the muscles and the increase in the strength and function after the period of mental training is the result of mental training on central motor planning [30]. Some other researchers have argued that mental training not only plays a role in the planning and design of movement, but also plays an important role in the implementation phase as well and is similar to physical exercise [31]. There are also different theories about how mental exercises affect motor learning and the development of motor skills. Meanwhile, psycho-neuro-muscular theory and symbolic learning theory can be used to justify the development of motor skills and postural control in the present study subjects. According to the psycho-neuromuscular theory, the mental exercise of the movement causes the activity potential and partial contraction of the muscles, and the motor feedbacks resulting from this partial muscle activity are transmitted to the brain centers and affect the activity of these areas, strengthens and improves coordination, and affect the motor learning as a result. This theory is consistent with Jacobson's theory that mental activity is essentially a weakened physical activity [30-32].

Non-declared

Non-declared

Mental practice is a practical and effective way to improve the dynamic control of students` posture in Amin Police University.

Thanks to all students of Amin Police University and staff members of the Physical Education Dept. of Amin Police University who helped us with this study.

Non-declared

Non-declared

Costs are provided by the authors.

TABLES and CHARTS

Show attach file


CITIATION LINKS

[1]Akuthota V, Nadler SF. Core strengthening. Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85(Suppl 1):86-92.
[2]Punakallio A. Balance abilities of workers in physically demanding jobs: With special reference to firefighters of different ages. J Sports Sci Med. 2005;4(8):1-47.
[3]Blackburn T, Guskiewicz KM, Petschaur MA, Prentice WE. Balance and joint stability: the relative contributions of proprioception and muscular strength. J Sport Rehabil. 2012;9(4):315-29.
[4]Spurrs RW, Murphy AJ, Watsford ML. The effect of plyometric training on distance running performance. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2003;89(1):1-7.
[5]Wikstrom EA, Tillman MD, Smith AN, Borsa PA. A new force-plate technology measure of dynamic postural stability. The dynamic postural stability index. J Athl Train. 2015;40(4):305-9.
[6]Salci Y, Kental BB, Heycan C, Akin S, Korkusuz F. Comparison of landing maneuvers between male and female college volleyball players. Clin Biomech. 2004;19(6):622-8.
[7]Boden BP, Dean GS, Feagin JA Jr, Garret WE Jr. Mechanisms of anterior cruciate ligament injury. Orthopedics. 2010;23(6):573-8.
[8] Gribble P, Hertel J, Denegar CR, Buckley WE. The effects of fatigue and chronic ankle instability on dynamic postural control. J Athl Train. 2004;39(4):321-9.
[9]Stane ML, Powers ME. The effects of plyometric training on selected measures of leg strength and power when compared to weight training and combination weight and plyometric training. J Athl Train. 2005;42(3):186-92.
[10]Weinberg RS, Gould D. Foundations of sport and exercise psychology. 2nd edition. Champaign, Illinois: Human Kinetics publishers; 1999. pp. 265-84.
[11]Barr K, Hall C. The use of imagery by Rowers. Int J Sport Psychol. 1970;23:243-61.
[12]Decety J, Jeannerod M, Germain M, Pastene J. Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort. Behav Brain Res. 1991;42(1):1-5.
[13]Cohen H, Heaton LG, Congdon SL, Jenkins HA. Changes in sensory organization tests scores with age. Age Ageing. 1996;25(1):39-44.
[14]Cumming J, Hall C. Deliberate imagery practice: The development of imagery skills in competitive athletes. J Sports Sci. 2002;20(2):137-45.
[15]Fery YA, Morizot P. Kinesthetic and visual image in modeling closed motor skills: The example of the tennis serve. Percept Mot Skills. 2000;90(3 Pt 1):707-22.
[16]Millard M, Mahoney C, Wardrop J. A preliminary study of mental and physical practice on the kayak wet exit skill. Percept Mot Skills. 2001;92(3 Pt 2):977-84.
[17]Roure R, Collet C, Deschaumes-Molinaro C, Delhomme G, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E. Imagery quality estimated by autonomic response in correlated to sporting performance enhancement. Physiol Behav. 1999;66(1):63-72.
[18]Yue G, Cole KJ. Strength increases form the motor program: Comparison of training with maximal voluntary and imagined muscle Contractions. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67(5):1114-23.
[19]Fairweather MM, Sideway B. Iseokinetic imagery as a postural development technique. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2013;64(4):385-92.
[20]Harwood C, Cumming J, Hall C. Imagery use in elite youth sport: Reinforcing the applied of achievement goal theory. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2012;74(3):292-300.
[21]Guillot A, Lebon F, Rouffet D, Champely S, Doyon J, Collet C. Muscular responses during motor imagery as a function of muscle contraction types. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;66(1):18-27.
[22] Crommond DJ. Motor imagery: Nerve in your wildest dream. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20(2):54-7.
[23]Decety J. The neurophy biological basis of motor imagery. Behav Brain Res. 1996;77(1-2):45-52.
[24]Gribble PA, Hertel J. Considerations for the normalizing measures of the star excursion balance test. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2003;7(2):89-100.
[25]Earl JE, Hertel J. Lower-extremity muscle activation during the star excursion balance tests. J Sport Rehabil. 2001;10(2):93-104.
[26]Niktab AR, Salari A. The effect of mental and physical practice on standing balance: The orthopaedic rehabilitation approach. J Kerman Univ Med Sci. 2003;10(3):172-9. [Persian]
[27]Decety J. Do imagine and executed actions share the same neural substrate?. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1996;3(2):87-93.
[28]Stephan KM, Frackowiak RSJ. Motor imagery: Anatomical representation and electrophysiological characteristics. Neurochem Res. 1996;21(9):1105-16.
[29]Yágüez L, Nagel D, Hoffman H, Canavan AG, Wist E, Hömberg V. A mental route to motor learning: Improving Trajectorial kinematics through imagery training. Behav Brain Res. 1998;90(1):95-106.
[30]Fansler CL, Poff CL, Shepard KF. Effect of mental practice on balance in elderly women. Phys Ther. 1985;65(9):1332-8.
[31]Gandevia SC, Wilson LR, Inglis JT, Burke D. Mental rehearsal of motor tasks recruits motoneurons but fails to recruit human fusimotor neurons selectively. J Physiol. 1997;505(1):259-66.
[32]Anett J. Motor imagery: Perception or action?. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33(11):1395-417.